Understanding the Olfactory Connection to Anxiety and Behavior
Written on
Chapter 1: The Impact of Olfactory Cues on Social Perception
When you begin to feel uneasy or anxious around others, even if their actions or words are not overtly visible, you might be sensing the subtle odor of fear emanating from those nearby. A personal experience during a presentation skills workshop illustrates this point vividly.
The session commenced early in a modest conference room at a local hotel. As the trainer entered, she took a seat beside me while waiting to be introduced. Almost immediately, I detected the faint scent of perspiration, a telltale sign of anxiety. This led me to wonder whether the trainer was feeling apprehensive about leading the training, despite her seemingly confident demeanor.
During the workshop, we were encouraged to participate in brief public speaking exercises. I volunteered to be the first speaker. Just as I began, the trainer interrupted, posing a question to the group: "Do you think she appears confident?" The participants looked perplexed by her inquiry. She continued, asserting, "I don't think so. Her voice pitch suggests otherwise." Feeling slightly irritated by her approach, I clarified that I was simply feeling more energized than usual, not anxious.
To my astonishment, she persisted in her interpretation, noting that my hand placement on the table was indicative of fear, suggesting I was seeking something to grasp. I insisted this had nothing to do with anxiety. As the session progressed, it became apparent that the trainer was highlighting the "nervous" nonverbal cues of other participants as well.
After the training, I spoke with the trainer, expressing my thoughts on her tendency to interpret speakers' behaviors as signs of anxiety, even when that might not be accurate. She countered by stating that many people mask their insecurity with a facade of confidence. In that moment, it struck me that she was projecting her own anxiety onto the participants. Consequently, feedback from attendees was lukewarm, with some feeling even more nervous about public speaking afterward.
What do you think transpired? Were the participants genuinely anxious, or was it the trainer who felt uncomfortable? Both parties were likely in a heightened state of arousal typical of public speaking situations. When the trainer sat next to me, her scent of perspiration could have heightened my own feelings of nervousness. Research on affective contagion—where emotions and moods are shared—suggests that fear can be transmitted not just through visual cues but also through smell. For example, a fearful airline passenger might unknowingly emit a scent that increases anxiety among those around them, even if they are typically calm flyers.
De Groot, Semin, and Smeets (2014) found that "olfactory fear signals were as potent as audiovisual fear signals in inducing fear, challenging the notion that human emotion communication is restricted to linguistic or visual channels."
The physiological responses involved in fear-induced sweating differ significantly from those resulting from physical exertion. The scent of fear is distinct from that of normal sweat due to the involvement of specific sweat glands that respond to anxiety, stress, or fear, producing an odorless secretion that takes on a characteristic smell only through bacterial breakdown.
Interestingly, the ability to perceive the scent of fear varies among ethnic groups. Research indicates that East Asians have a less detectable body odor than individuals of European or African descent.
Our instinct to perceive and mimic the emotions and behaviors of others operates automatically and unconsciously. Research by Hatfield, Cacioppo, and Rapson indicates that emotional contagion typically flows from individuals of higher status to their followers. Additionally, negative emotions tend to spread more readily than positive ones due to our biological predisposition to focus on threats.
In the training scenario, the trainer likely held a higher status within the group, giving her emotional state the power to influence the participants. This means that some attendees may have been reflecting her anxiety rather than displaying their own. If the trainer had recognized this dynamic, she might have avoided misinterpreting her participants' mental states.
Another factor contributing to her misinterpretation could be the attentional focus influenced by affective states. Anxious individuals often have a bias towards recognizing fearful expressions and interpreting neutral faces through the lens of their emotions.
Understanding this dynamic could have enlightened the trainer to the fact that while participants appeared neutral, she might have misjudged their feelings as signs of anxiety. It’s important to note that while she may not have experienced anxiety in general, she might have faced anxiety within the context of the training.
To mitigate misinterpretations of behaviors and emotions, it's crucial to acknowledge our own feelings rather than suppress them. Moderate anxiety can even enhance performance, as highlighted in research by Chen, which indicated that human fear signals can boost cognitive function.
As discussed in our book, Mastering Mentalization, becoming aware of our affective states is essential. Our mental states shape not only our behavior but also how we perceive others. Basic mentalizing competencies can enhance our understanding of these dynamics.
Basic mentalizing refers to our fundamental ability to recognize and share social signals through mimicry and emotional contagion. Developing these competencies could have helped the trainer avoid the misinterpretations that occurred, allowing her to focus on the true challenges faced by her participants. This, in turn, would have fostered a positive learning environment and encouraged more constructive feedback, thereby enhancing her own confidence for future sessions.
Can you recall a time when you absorbed the emotional state of someone else? What were the results? How might things have turned out differently if you'd had this knowledge at the time?
In this insightful video, we explore how fear can be communicated through scent, shedding light on the intricate connections between olfactory signals and emotional states.
Chapter 2: Analyzing the Olfactory Signals of Fear
This video features the soundtrack "The Smell of Fear" from Puss in Boots: The Last Wish, illustrating the emotional depth and atmosphere of fear in storytelling.
References
- De Groot, J. H. B., Semin, G. R., & Smeets, M. A. M. (2014). I can see, hear, and smell your fear: Comparing olfactory and audiovisual media in fear communication. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(2), 825–834. doi:10.1037/a0033731
- Wilke, K., Martin, A., Terstegen, L., Biel, S. S. (June 2007). A short history of sweat gland biology. International Journal of Cosmetic Science, 29(3): 169–179. doi:10.1111/j.1467–2494.2007.00387.x
- Preti, G., & Leyden, J. J. (2010). Genetic Influences on Human Body Odor: From Genes to the Axillae. Journal of Investigative Dermatology, 130(2), 344–346. doi:10.1038/jid.2009.396
- Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional Contagion. Cambridge (UK): Cambridge University Press.
- Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: The role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive Affective Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1): 52–63.
- Yoon, K. L., & Zinbarg, R. E. (2008). Interpreting Neutral Faces as Threatening Is a Default Mode for Socially Anxious Individuals. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 117(3): 680–685. doi: 10.1037/0021–843X.117.3.680
- Chen, D. (2006). Chemosignals of Fear Enhance Cognitive Performance in Humans. Chemical Senses, 31(5), 415–423. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjj046
- van der Putten, A. A. J. T. (2022). Mastering Mentalization. ToM PRESS.